What Goes In
Must Come Out:
Children's Media Violence Consumption at Home and
Aggressive Behaviors at School
Audrey M. Buchanan (Brigham Young University)
Douglas A. Gentile, Ph.D. (National Institute on
Media and the Family)
David A. Nelson, Ph.D. (Brigham Young University)
David A. Walsh, Ph.D. (National Institute on Media
and the Family)
Julia Hensel (St. Mary's University)
Paper presented at the International Society
for the Study of Behavioural Development Conference,
Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. (2002, August)
Introduction
A significant number of studies have solidified
the notion that media violence has an effect on
children's subsequent aggression (see Bensley
& Eenwyk, 2001 and Wilson, Smith, Potter, Kunkel,
Linz, Colvin, & Donnerstein, 2002, for recent
reviews). Violent media can take many forms, ranging
from television programming and movies to video
games and other interactive activities. This study
examines a number of these different media formats.
Previous studies of the impact of media violence
on childhood aggression have been limited by a
focus on physical forms of aggression, which tend
to be more common among boys. Accordingly, little
is known of the impact of media violence on aggression
in girls.
This study expands upon previous research by
examining subtypes of aggression in relation to
violent media. In particular, research has established
relational aggression as a point of contrast with
physical forms of aggression (see Crick et al.,
1999, for a review). Children who spread rumors,
exclude peers, and engage in other relationship-oriented
aggression are different than those who simply
hit or kick to aggress against another. Relational
aggression has been defined as "behaviors that
harm others through damage (or the threat of damage)
to relationships or feelings of acceptance, friendship,
or group inclusion" (Crick, 1996). Studies show
that relational aggression is associated with
a significant level of negative consequences for
both perpetrators and their victims (see Crick
et al., 1999, for a review).
Physical and relational forms of aggression are
moderately correlated, which is to be expected
(given that they are both forms of aggressive
behavior). Nonetheless, relational aggression
emerges as a distinct form of aggression and studies
have begun to focus on the possible differential
correlates of these subtypes (Crick et al., 1999).
To our knowledge, no study has yet compared physically
and relationally aggressive children in terms
of their violent media viewing habits. Accordingly,
the current research separately considered both
forms of aggression.
Research regarding the media violence and aggressive
behavior has often been hampered by emphasis on
the question of whether media violence actually
promotes aggressive behavior or, in contrast,
whether aggressive children simply prefer media
violence, consistent with their behavioral style.
Research tends to suggest that both processes
are in motion, and the effects of media violence
are indeed heightened for children who already
struggle with aggressive tendencies (Huesmann
& Miller, 1994; Coie & Dodge, 1998). Accordingly,
analyses in this study focus on highly aggressive
children, using an extreme groups approach, in
order to define the possible impact of media violence
on children who are most at risk for developmental
difficulties.
A second area of interest in the current study
is that of social information processing styles
related to exposure to media violence. In particular,
we were interested in the possible relation of
media violence to the formation of intent attributions.
Previous research has demonstrated that the association
between hostile attributional bias and social
maladjustment is quite strong, and has been demonstrated
with children of all ages (see Crick & Dodge,
1994, for a review). In particular, physically
aggressive children tend to exhibit a hostile
attributional bias, in which they tend to infer
hostile intent from the actions of others, even
when intent is ambiguous and might be benign.
This style of processing understandably contributes
to the development and maintenance of aggressive
behavior. This research is also limited in regard
to consideration of aggressive girls, as the focus
of such studies tends to be aggressive boys. In
contrast, Crick (1995) has shown that relationally
aggressive children also tend to exhibit hostile
attributional biases, although social context
matters a lot. In particular, Crick (1995) demonstrated
that instrumental conflicts (e.g., a peer breaking
your toy) are more salient and provocative for
physically aggressive children whereas relational
conflicts (e.g., a peer fails to invite you to
his birthday party) tend to elicit a response
consistent with a hostile attributional bias in
relationally aggressive children. Social information-processing
theory suggests that violent media might activate
cognitive structures, "making it more likely that
other incoming information would be processed
in an 'aggression' framework, possibly increasing
aggressive behavior" (Bensley & Eenwyk, 2001).
Considering many children seem to be predisposed
to assume hostility in ambiguous situations, violent
media has the potential to be a destructive contributing
factor. Thus, we examined relationships between
violent media habits and hostile attributional
bias (for instrumental and relational conflict
situations) in the current study.
Method
Participants
Two hundred and nineteen 3rd (n = 81), 4th (n
= 49), and 5th grade (n= 89) students participated
in the study. Students were recruited from four
Minnesota schools, including one suburban private
school (n = 41), two suburban public schools (n
= 151), and one rural public school (n = 27).
The sample was almost evenly divided between boys
and girls, with 49% of the children being female
(51% male). Participants ranged in age from 7
to 11 years of age (M = 9.53; SD = 1.07). Ninety-one
percent of the respondents classified their ethnic
background as Caucasian (which is representative
of the region). Participants were treated in accordance
with the "Ethical Principles of Psychologists
and Code of Conduct" (American Psychological Association,
1992).
Procedure
Data were collected between November 2000 and
June 2001. Letters were mailed directly to the
parents of students in participating classrooms
informing them about the study and requesting
consent. Consent levels were greater than 70%
for all classrooms. Interested teachers volunteered
their classrooms for inclusion in the study. Each
of the participating classrooms was a mandatory
class (i.e., not elective) to reduce the likelihood
of self-selection bias.
Each participant completed three confidential
surveys:
- a peer-nomination measure of aggressive and
prosocial behaviors,
- a self-report survey of media habits and demographic
data, and
- a self-report measure of hostile attribution
bias.
Trained research personnel administered the peer-nomination
survey, and classroom teachers were trained to
administer the other surveys. The surveys were
administered on consecutive days. Teachers also
completed one survey for each participating child,
reporting on the frequency of children's aggressive
and prosocial behaviors.
Assessment of Social Adjustment
A peer nomination instrument was utilized in
order to assess children's social adjustment,
and was adapted from a peer nomination instrument
that has been used in several previous studies
of children's social behavior (e.g. Crick, 1995;
Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). This instrument consists
of 10 items. Two of these items were the peer
sociometric items (nominations of liked and disliked
peers), which are used extensively in research
of this nature to assess peer acceptance and rejection
(see Crick & Dodge, 1994) for a review. The remaining
8 items assess four different types of social
behavior: physical aggression (2 item subscale),
relational aggression (3 item subscale), prosocial
behavior (2 item subscale), and verbal aggression
(1 item). For the purposes of this study, the
physical aggression and relational aggression
subscales were examined (see Table 1 for a listing
of all of items related to the different subscales).
Cronbach's alpha was computed for each of the
three subscales and was found to be satisfactory:
a = .93 for physical aggression, a = .86 for relational
aggression, and a = .81 for prosocial behavior.
Children's physical and relational aggression
scores were used as continuous variables in subsequent
correlational analyses and were also used to identify
groups of aggressive and nonaggressive children
(for categorical comparison). In particular, the
classification of aggressive groups was based
on an extreme groups approach. Children with scores
one standard deviation above the mean were considered
aggressive, and the remaining children were classified
as nonaggressive. This allowed for the organization
of four groups:
- non-aggressive (both relational and physical
aggression scores low),
- physically aggressive (relational aggression
low, physical aggression high),
- relationally aggressive (relational aggression
high, physical aggression low) and
- combined relationally and physically aggressive
(both scores high).
Teacher Ratings of Aggressive Behavior
Teachers completed a survey assessing children's
aggression and prosocial behavior for each child
participating in the study. This instrument consists
of twelve behavioral subscales, including a variety
of behaviors (e.g. aggressive behavior, victimization,
prosocial behavior, and others). For the purposes
of this study, only the subscales reflecting relational
aggression and physical aggression are used in
subsequent analyses. These items are listed in
Table 2. Cronbach's alpha was computed and found
to be satisfactory for each subscale: a = .93
for teacher ratings of relational aggression and
a = .94 for teacher ratings of physical aggression.
Finally, similar to the peer-nomination measure,
the continuous scores for each of these subscales
was used to identify groups of children according
to aggressive status (as described above).
Assessment of Media Habits
Violent media exposure Similar to Anderson
and Dill's (2000) approach, participants were
asked to name their three favorite television
shows, their three favorite video or computer
games, and their three favorite movies/videos.
For each named media product, participants were
asked to rate how frequently they watched or played
on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = "Almost never,"
5 = "Almost every day"). Participants were also
asked to rate how violent they consider each media
product to be on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = "Not
at all violent," 4 = "Very violent"). A violence
exposure score was computed for each participant
by multiplying the frequency of watching or playing
each media product by its subjective violence
rating, and then taking the mean of the three
similar products. Accordingly, media-specific
(i.e., Violent TV Exposure, Violent Video Game
Exposure, and Violent Movies/Videos Exposure)
violence exposure scores were computed for each
participant. Finally, an overall violent media
exposure score, the mean of all nine products
(TV, video games, movies/videos), was also calculated.
Previous research has confirmed that participants
were likely to assess the violence in media products
based on the amount of physical violence, rather
than relational aggression. People's ratings were
most strongly correlated with the graphicness
of the portrayal of physical violence, across
age, gender, amount of television viewing, and
other factors (Potter, 1999).
Preference for violent video games.
One item assessed each participant's preference
for more or less violent video games by asking,
"On a scale from 1 to 5, how much violence do
you like to have in video games?"
Amount of television watching and video game
play
Participants provided the amount of time they
spent watching television and playing video games
during different time periods on weekdays and
weekends. Weekly amounts were calculated from
these responses.
Assessment of hostile attributional bias/social
information processing.
The final survey was an adapted version of a
hostile attribution survey that has been reliably
used in past research (e.g., Crick, 1995; Nelson
& Crick, 1999). This instrument is composed of
10 stories, each describing an instance of provocation
in which the intent of the provocateur is ambiguous.
The stories were developed to reflect common situations
that children and young adolescents might encounter
in the school years. Four of the stories depict
instrumental provocations and six represent relational
provocations. Participants answer two questions
following each story. The first presents four
possible reasons for the peer's behavior, two
of which indicate hostile intent and two reflect
benign intent. The second question asks whether
the provocateur(s) intended to be mean or not.
This survey relates to the participant's perception
of hostility from the outside world. Two scale
scores result from analysis of this measure: intent
attributions for relational provocation and intent
attributions for instrumental provocations.
Based on procedures delineated by Fitzgerald
and Asher (1987), the children's responses to
the attribution assessments were summed within
and across the stories for each provocation type.
Possible scores ranged from 0 through 12 (0-8
for the instrumental subscale and 0-12 for the
relational subscale). Finally, Cronbach's alpha
was computed for each of these scales and found
to be satisfactory: intent attributions for relational
provocations (a = .81) and intent attributions
for instrumental provocations (a = .74).
Results
The first set of analyses report simple bivariate
correlations for all of the variables included
in the study. Further analyses considered grade,
sex, and aggression group differences for the
various media variables. These analyses were conducted
separately for teacher ratings and peer behavioral
nominations of aggressive behavior and, as noted
earlier, discriminate between physical and relational
forms of childhood aggression.
Correlation Findings
Teacher and peer ratings were generally consistent
with each other across the various correlations
(see Table 3). Interestingly, amount of television
viewing correlated with not only exposure to violent
television content, violent video games, violent
movies/videos and the violent media index, but
more importantly, with a preference for violence
in video games and peer reports of physical aggression.
Similarly, amount of video game play per week
was significantly associated with exposure to
violent television content, violent video games,
violent movies/videos and the violent media index.
It was also correlated with a preference for violent
video games and both peer and teacher reports
of physical aggression. In general, children with
greater exposure to violent media preferred more
violent video games.
The hostile attribution scores also showed many
significant correlations. Instrumental hostile
attribution was significantly correlated with
a preference for violent video games, amount of
television watched per week, the violent television
exposure index, the violent video game exposure
index, the violent media exposure index, the relational
hostile attribution scores, and peer ratings of
relational aggression.
Likewise, the relational hostile attribution
scores were associated with a preference for violent
video games, the violent media exposure index,
and the violent video game exposure index. (See
Table 3 for all correlation findings.)
Findings Related to Teacher Ratings of Aggressions
To assess grade, sex, and physical and relational
aggression group differences in children's exposure
to and preference for violent media, 3 (grade)
x 2 (sex) x 2 (physical aggression: aggressive
or nonaggressive) x 2 (relational aggression:
aggressive or nonaggressive) analyses of variance
were conducted. Variables relating to children's
exposure to and preference for different forms
of violent media served as the dependent variables.
In regards to preference for violence in their
video games, analyses produced significant main
effects for grade, F(2, 193) = 6.6, p < .01, sex,
F(1, 193) = 85.1, p < .001, physical aggression,
F(1, 193) = 4.5, p < .05 and relational aggression,
F(1, 193) = 9.4, p < .01. Specifically, a post-hoc
test (Fisher's LSD) (p < .05) revealed that the
fifth graders (M=2.7, SD=1.2) were significantly
more likely than fourth graders (M=2.1, SD= 1.0)
to prefer violence in their video games. In regard
to the main effect for sex, boys (M=3.1, SD=1.1)
were more likely to favor video game violence
than girls (M=1.8, SD=1.0). In addition, physically
aggressive children (M=3.2, SD=1.2) and relationally
aggressive children (M=2.7, SD=1.3) also tended
to favor more violence in their video game play
over their nonaggressive peers (M=2.3, SD=1.2;
M=2.4, SD=1.2, respectively).
The analyses of the violent television exposure
index revealed significant main effects for sex,
F(1, 197) = 36.9, p < .001 and relational aggression,
F(1, 197) = 8.3, p < .01. In addition, a significant
sex X relational aggression interaction F(1, 197)
= 5.2, p < .05, and a physical aggression X relational
aggression interaction F(1, 197) = 6.6, p < .05
were found. Analysis of the means showed that
boys (M=6.2, SD=2.6) were exposed to relatively
more violent television than girls (M=4.4, SD=1.7).
In addition, relationally aggressive children
(M=5.9, SD=2.6) report being exposed to significantly
more violent television programming than their
nonaggressive peers (M=5.2, SD=2.3). Furthermore,
a simple effects of analysis of variance of the
relational aggression group means (conducted separately
by gender) showed the main effect of the relationally
aggressive group to be significant for boys only,
F(1, 108) = 11.1, p < .01. Specifically, relationally
aggressive boys (M = 8.4, SD = 2.6) were significantly
more likely to be exposed to violent television
than nonaggressive boys (M = 5.9, SD = 2.5). Finally,
a post hoc test (Fisher's LSD) (p< .05) conducted
on the physical aggression X relational aggression
means found that children who were comorbid for
aggressive behavior (both physically and relationally
aggressive, M=6.6, SD=2.4) were more likely to
be exposed to violent television programming than
nonaggressive children (M=5.1, SD=2.3).
For the violent video games exposure index, analyses
demonstrated a significant main effect for sex,
F(1, 182) = 48.2, p < .001, and relational aggression,
F(1, 182) = 5.2, p < .05. In regard to the sex
effect, boys (M= 6.7, SD = 3.7) were more likely
to report greater exposure to violent video game
play than girls (M = 3.6, SD =2.5). In addition,
relationally aggressive children (M = 5.9, SD
= 3.8) were more involved in violent video game
play than their nonaggressive peers (M = 5.2,
SD = 3.5).
For the violent movie/video exposure index, analyses
showed a significant main effect for sex, F(1,
195) = 16.9, p < .001. Analysis of the means showed
that boys (M = 4.6, SD = 2.8) were more likely
to be exposed to violent movies and videos than
girls (M = 3.3, SD = 2.0).
For the violent media exposure index, analyses
revealed significant main effects for sex, F(1,
199) = 63.6, p < .001, and relational aggression,
F(1, 199) = 6.6, p < .05. In regard to the main
effect for sex, boys (M = 5.9, SD = 2.3) reported
greater exposure than girls (M= 3.8, SD = 1.6)
to all forms of media violence. In addition, relationally
aggressive children (M = 5.2, SD = 2.2) also reported
significantly more exposure to overall media violence
than their nonaggressive peers (M = 4.8, SD =
2.2). Finally, analyses revealed a physical aggression
X relational aggression interaction, F(1, 199)
= 7.9, p < .01. A post-hoc test (Fisher's LSD)
(p< .05) conducted on the physical aggression
X relational aggression means found that children
who were physically aggressive (M=6.1, SD=2.6)
were more likely to be exposed to violent media
than nonaggressive children (M=4.6, SD=2.1).
Findings Relating to Peer Ratings of Aggression
To assess grade, sex, and physical and relational
aggression group differences in children's exposure
to and preference for violent media, 3 (grade)
x 2 (sex) x 2 (physical aggression: aggressive
or nonaggressive) x 2 (relational aggression:
aggressive or nonaggressive) analyses of variance
were conducted. Variables relating to children's
exposure to and preference for different forms
of violent media served as the dependent variables.
In regards to preference for violence in their
video games, analyses revealed significant main
effects for grade, F(2, 192) = 7.2, p < .001,
sex, F(1, 192) = 86.7, p < .001, and relational
aggression, F(1, 192) = 10.1, p < .01. As for
the main effect for grade, post-hoc tests (Fisher's
LSD) (p< .05) showed that both 3rd graders (M
= 2.5, SD = 1.2) and 5th graders (M = 2.7, SD
= 1.2) were significantly more likely to prefer
violence in their video games than 4th graders
(M = 2.1, SD = 1.0). In addition, boys (M = 3.1,
SD = 1.1) were more likely to prefer violence
than girls (M = 1.8, SD = 1.0). In regard to the
main effect for relational aggression, relationally
aggressive children (M = 3.0, SD = 1.4) showed
a greater preference for violence than their nonaggressive
peers (M = 2.4, SD = 1.2).
For the violent television exposure index, analyses
demonstrated significant main effects for sex,
F(1, 196) = 37.0, p < .001, physical aggression,
F(1, 196) = 7.3, p < .01, and relational aggression,
F(1, 196) = 4.2, p < .05. There was also a significant
grade x sex interaction, F(2, 196) = 3.4, p <
.05. In regard to the main effect for sex, boys
(M = 6.8, SD = 3.8) were much more likely to report
exposure to violent television than girls (M =
3.6, SD = 2.5). In addition, both physically aggressive
children (M = 6.8, SD = 2.8) and relationally
aggressive children (M = 6.2, SD = 2.6) were exposed
to significantly higher levels of televised violence
than nonaggressive children (M = 5.1, SD = 2.3)
for physical aggression comparison; M = 5.2, SD
= 2.3 for relational comparison).
Analyses of the violent video games exposure
index showed significant main effects for grade,
F(2, 181) = 3.2, p < .05, sex, F(1, 181) = 48.5,
p < .001, and relational aggression, F(1, 181)
= 5.4, p < .05. Post-hoc analyses of the means
(Fisher's LSD) (p < .05) showed that 5th graders
(M = 6.1, SD = 4.2) had significantly more exposure
to violent video games than their 3rd and 4th
grade counterparts (M = 5.0, SD = 3.3; M = 4.8,
SD = 3.0, respectively). In addition, boys (M
= 6.8, SD = 3.9) were far more likely than girls
to be exposed to violent video games (M = 3.6,
SD = 2.5). Finally, relationally aggressive children
(M = 6.5, SD = 4.4) were more likely to play violent
video games than their nonaggressive peers (M
= 5.2, SD = 3.5).
Analyses of the violent movie/video exposure
index revealed significant main effects for sex,
F(1, 194) = 17.4, p < .001 and physical aggression,
F(1, 194) = 6.5, p < .05. Analysis of the means
showed that boys (M = 4.7, SD = 3.0) were more
likely to watch violent movies and videos than
girls (M = 3.3, SD = 2.0). In addition, physically
aggressive children (M = 5.5, SD = 4.0) were also
more likely than non-physically aggressive children
(M = 3.8, SD = 2.4) to watch violent movies and
videos
Finally, analyses of the violent media exposure
index revealed significant main effects for grade,
F(2, 198) = 3.5, p < .05, sex, F(1, 198) = 62.6,
p < 001, physical aggression status, F(1, 198)
= 6.0, p < .05, and relational aggression group,
F(1, 198) = 7.2, p < .01. Post-hoc tests of the
grade means (Fisher's LSD) (p < .05) showed that
5th graders (M = 5.3, SD = 2.7) were more likely
to be exposed to violent media of all types than
their 3rd and 4th grade peers (M = 4.7, SD = 1.9;
M = 4.5, SD = 2.1, respectively). In regard to
the main effect for sex, boys (M = 6.0, SD = 2.4)
reported significantly more exposure to all types
of violent media than girls (M = 3.8, SD = 1.6).
In addition, physically aggressive children (M
= 6.3, SD = 2.7) reported greater exposure than
non-physically aggressive children (M = 4.7, SD
= 2.2) and relationally aggressive children (M
= 5.8, SD = 2.5) also reported greater exposure
than non-relationally aggressive peers (M = 4.7,
SD = 2.2).
Discussion
To date, this is the first research conducted
regarding relational aggression and media violence.
The true seriousness of relational aggression
and its potential destructive effects are just
beginning to be recognized in the field, and much
exploration of this exciting area is sure to follow
these harbinger efforts. Another strength of the
study is that it incorporates authentic measures
of aggression, teacher and peer reports, which
have strong ecological validity.
Findings revealed that children who watched more
television and played video games more often were
more likely to view violence and exhibit hostile
attributional biases. Perhaps those spending more
time engaged in these media forms have less parent
supervision of their activities and viewing material,
and the children are left to their own devices.
Secondarily, perhaps these children are inadvertently
exposed to television violence, due to the sheer
number of hours they report spending with these
media forms.
Hostile attributions were associated with multiple
indices of exposure to violent media and teacher
and peer ratings of violent behavior. It appears
that those children who engage in violent media
viewing and play tend to assume the worst in their
interactions with others. While the direction
of effect is not clear, this finding merits additional
investigation.
The sex difference was strong across the various
findings. Boys were exposed to more violent media
and preferred more violent media. The reasons
for this might include socialization differences-the
toys, games and even subtle messages boys and
girls receive guide their behavior and what they
expect of themselves. This finding has implications
for the importance of prudence and care in the
socialization of young children. This includes
what they are exposed to in terms of media.
In regards to their preference for violent media
forms, fifth graders consistently showed greater
preference than either their fourth grade counterparts
alone or sometimes both third and fourth grade
children. The implications of this apparent increase
with age are addressed further on.
Relationally aggressive children were shown to
view and play more violent media than their nonaggressive
peers. This provides a persuasive case for the
idea that violent media does not only contribute
to physical aggression, but that it is possible
that subtleties in media character relationships
demonstrate other ways that individuals hurt one
another.
What do these relational aggression findings
mean? It is possible that children who indulge
in relational aggression perceive it as more subtle
and easier to perpetrate without significant repercussions
from parents or teachers. This may be why relational
aggression often emerged as significant without
physical aggression. Another possibility might
be that currently comorbid children began with
relational aggression for the same self-defensive
reasons and then moved on to more overt, physical
forms of aggression.
Of course, there were limitations in the study.
Children's ratings of the violence in their favorite
media were subjective. It is possible that children
who view violence more frequently might report
it as being less violent, having been desensitized
to its content over time. Conversely, children
who view violence infrequently might inflate their
reports of the violence they do observe.
Also, the findings reported here are correlational
and do not merit casual assessment. However, the
significant level of consistency of the findings
give substantial evidence for the central purpose
of this report-to document differences in children's
aggression, based upon their violent media consumption.
Future analyses will include the factor of parental
involvement, conceptualized as whether or not
parents place limits on children's consumption
of different kinds of media and how often a parent
watches television programs with their child (providing
opportunities for discussion of the various scenes
portrayed). These will provide a more detailed
picture of the results given here.
It is likely that the perpetration of relational
aggression increases as children grow into young
adolescents and peer groups become increasingly
important to them (Nelson & Crick, 1999). Closer
examination of popular media is needed in order
to assess its danger in terms of relational aggression.
This research underscores the need for continued
study of these relationships. It will have implications
for parents and educators alike in the prevention
of aggression problems, and possibly inform future
interventions with maladjusted children.
References
Anderson, C. A.; Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games
and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior
in the laboratory and in life. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 7,
772-791.
Bensley, L., & Eenwyk, J. (2001). Video games
and real-life aggression: review of the literature.
Journal of Adolescent
Health, 29 vol. 3). New York: Wiley and
Sons.
Coie, J. D. & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression
and antisocial behavior. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
Handbook of
Child Psychology (5th ed., vol. 3). New
York: Wiley and Sons.
Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression,
relational aggression, and prosocial behavior
in children's future social adjustment. Child
Development, 67, 2317-2327.
Crick, N. R., Werner, N. E., Casas, J. F., O'Brien,
K. M., Nelson, D.A., Grotpeter, J. K., & Markon,
K. (1999). Childhood aggression and gender: A
new look at an old problem. In D. Bernstein (Ed.),
Volume 45 of
the Nebraska symposium on motivation: Gender and
motivation. (pp. 75-141). Lincoln, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.
Crick, N., and Grotpeter, J. (1995). Relational
aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment.
Child Development,
66, 710-722.
Crick, N. and Dodge, K. (1994). A review and
reformulation of social information-processing
mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological
Bulletin, 115, 74-101.
Fitzgerald, P., & Asher, S. R. (1987, August).
Aggressive-rejected children's attributional biases
about liked and disliked peers. Paper presented
at the annual meeting of the American Psychological
Association, New York.
Fleming, M. & Rickwood, D. (2001). Effects of
violent versus nonviolent video games on children's
arousal, aggressive mood, and positive mood. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 2047-2071.
Funk, J., Hagan, J., Schimming, J., Bullock,
W., Buchman, D., & Myers, M. (2002). Aggression
and psychopathology in adolescents with a preference
for violent electronic games. Aggressive
Behavior, 28, 134-144.
Huesmann, L. R., & Miller, L. S. (1994). Long-term
effects of repeated exposure to media violence
in childhood. In L. R. Huesmann (Ed.), Aggressive
behavior: Current perspectives, 153-186,
New York: Plenum Press.
Nelson, D. A. and Crick, N. R. (1999). Rose-colored
glasses: examining the social information-processing
of prosocial young adolescents. Journal
of Early Adolescence, 19, 17-38.
Potter, W. James. On Media Violence. Thousand
Oaks, Calif: Sage Publications, 1999
Wilson, B., Smith, S., Potter, W., Kunkel, D.,
Linz, D., Colvin, C., & Donnerstein, E. (2002).
Violence in children's television programming:
assessing the risks. Journal
of Communication, 52, 5-35.
Table 1. Peer nomination subscale items
Physical aggression subscale:
- Who hits, kicks, or punches others?
- Who pushes and shoves other kids around?
Relational aggression subscale:
- Who tries to make another kid not like a certain
person by spreading rumors about them or talking
behind their backs?
- Who, when they are mad at a person, get even
by keeping that person from being in their group
of friends?
- Who, when they are mad at a person, ignore
the person or stop talking to them?
Verbal aggression item:
- Find the number of three kids who say mean
things to other kids to insult them or put them
down.
Prosocial behavior subscale:
- Who does nice things for others?
- Who tries to cheer up other kids who are upset
or sad about something? They try to make the
kids feel happy again.
Table 2. Teacher rating subscale items used in
this study.
Physical aggression subscale:
- This child hits or kicks peers.
- This child initiates or gets into physical
fights with peers.
- This child threatens to hit or beat up other
children.
- This child pushes or shoves peers.
Relational aggression subscale:
- When this child is mad at a peer, s/he gets
even by excluding the peer from his or her clique
or playgroup.
- This child spreads rumors or gossips about
some peers.
- When angry at a peer, this child tries to
get other children to stop playing with the
peer or to stop liking the peer.
- This child threatens to stop being a peer's
friend in order to hurt the peer or to get what
s/he wants from the peer.
- When mad at a peer, this child ignores the
peer or stops talking to the peer.
Table 3. Correlational table for variables used
in this study.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
5
|
6
|
7
|
8
|
9
|
10
|
11
|
12
|
13
|
1. Video Game (VG) Violence
Preference
|
1
|
.354**
|
.421**
|
.504**
|
.620**
|
.361**
|
.632**
|
.246**
|
.171*
|
.131
|
.325**
|
.012
|
.270**
|
2. Amt. TV per week
|
.354**
|
1
|
.467**
|
.313**
|
.270**
|
.288**
|
.373**
|
.163*
|
.120
|
.044
|
.160*
|
.064
|
.106
|
3. Amt. VG play per week
|
.421**
|
.467**
|
1
|
.378**
|
.628**
|
.345**
|
.602**
|
.107
|
.138
|
.044
|
.185**
|
-.108
|
.157*
|
4. Violent TV Expos. Index
|
.504**
|
.313**
|
.378**
|
1
|
.503**
|
.438**
|
.775**
|
.164*
|
.102
|
.200**
|
.300**
|
.140*
|
.214**
|
5. Violent VG Expo Index
|
.620**
|
.270**
|
.628**
|
.503**
|
1
|
.444**
|
.857**
|
.210**
|
.205**
|
.134
|
.221**
|
.072
|
.267**
|
6. Vio. Movie/Video Expo.
Index
|
.361**
|
.288**
|
.345**
|
.438**
|
.444**
|
1
|
.755**
|
.111
|
.102
|
.135*
|
.243**
|
.037
|
.216**
|
7. Vio. Media Expo. Index
|
.632**
|
.373**
|
.602**
|
.755**
|
.857**
|
.755**
|
1
|
.201**
|
.167**
|
.191**
|
.320**
|
.091
|
.297**
|
8. Instrumental Hostile Attribution
(HA)
|
.246**
|
.163**
|
.107
|
.164*
|
.210**
|
.111
|
.201**
|
1
|
.368**
|
.134*
|
.130
|
.121
|
.183**
|
9. Relational HA
|
.171*
|
.120
|
.138
|
.102
|
.205**
|
.102
|
.167*
|
.368**
|
1
|
.028
|
.091
|
.073
|
.130
|
10. PeerBRelational Aggression
Scale
|
.131
|
.044
|
.044
|
.200**
|
.134
|
.135*
|
.191**
|
.134*
|
.028
|
1
|
.642**
|
.497**
|
.317**
|
11. Peer-Physical Aggression
Scale
|
.325**
|
.160*
|
.185**
|
.300**
|
.221**
|
.243**
|
.320**
|
.130
|
.091
|
.624**
|
1
|
.296**
|
.529**
|
12. Teacher-Relational Aggression
|
.012
|
.064
|
-.108
|
.140*
|
.072
|
.037
|
.091
|
.121
|
.073
|
.497**
|
.296**
|
1
|
.313**
|
13. Teacher-Physical Aggression
|
.270**
|
.106
|
.157*
|
.214**
|
.267**
|
.216**
|
.297**
|
.183**
|
.130
|
.317**
|
.529**
|
.313**
|
1
|
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level.
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level.